Bibliografía - artículo

In recent years, the use of Student Response System (SRS) technology in higher education settings has gained popularity as research demonstrates its effectiveness in achieving desired outcomes, especially when paired with peer interaction. This article explores students’ perceptions of integrating the Game-based Student Response System (GSRS) Kahoot! in two Spanish flipped classrooms, supported by the Peer Instruction (PI) technique, at a university in Singapore. Adopting an action research approach, quantitative and qualitative data from 32 undergraduates were gathered through a questionnaire. The findings revealed that students perceived the use of Kahoot! to be beneficial in terms of: (1) increasing engagement and motivation to learn; (2) improving the understanding of concepts; and (3) contributing to a positive learning environment. Moreover, students found that PI, in particular, contributed to deepening their grammatical and pragmatic knowledge of Spanish. Implications that may be useful to language teachers are also presented.

----------------

En años recientes, el uso de tecnologías de Sistema de Respuesta del Estudiante (SRE) en entornos de educación superior ha ganado popularidad por la efectividad mostrada para lograr los resultados deseados, especialmente, cuando se combina con la interacción entre pares. Este artículo explora las percepciones de los estudiantes a raíz de la integración del Sistema de Respuesta del Estudiante basado en Juegos (SREJ) Kahoot! en dos clases de aula invertida de español como lengua extranjera o segunda (ELE/EL2), apoyado por la técnica de Instrucción entre Pares (IP), en una universidad de Singapur. Mediante un enfoque de investigación-acción, se recopilaron datos cuantitativos y cualitativos de 32 estudiantes universitarios mediante un cuestionario. Los resultados mostraron que los estudiantes percibieron el uso de Kahoot! beneficioso para: (1) aumentar el compromiso y la motivación para aprender; (2) mejorar la comprensión conceptual; y (3) contribuir a crear un ambiente positivo de aprendizaje. Además, los estudiantes encontraron que la instrucción entre pares en particular contribuyó a profundizar su conocimiento gramatical y pragmático del español. Se presentan también algunas implicaciones que pueden ser útiles para docentes de lenguas.

Texto completo (HTML)
Texto completo (PDF)

Texto completo pdf icon

Despite the growing interest in incorporating textual glosses into second language (L2) reading in hypermedia and paper media, no agreements have been reached as to whether and what extent a textual gloss facilitates L2 learners’ vocabulary acquisition in hypermedia and paper media. The present study meta-analysed the overall effects of textual glosses and different glossing types (i.e. language, location, and mode) on L2 vocabulary acquisition, as well as the moderation effects on the variability of L2 vocabulary acquisition effect sizes. Based on a comprehensive search, 20 studies involving 2,291 participants that have quantitatively documented the changes of L2 vocabulary gains through a pre-and-post textual glosses design were identified and coded in order to retrieve moderator variables. The results show the following: (1) textual glossing has a medium effect on L2 vocabulary acquisition; (2) the effect of textual glosses on L2 vocabulary acquisition persists over time; (3) glossing location and glossing mode significantly impact the effect of textual glosses on L2 vocabulary acquisition but glossing language has no definite effect; and (4) outcome measure is the only moderator variable among the three influencing factors on vocabulary acquisition. These results are further discussed with reference to existing studies and theory of second language acquisition.

This study measured the impact of explicit instruction on accent usage in the written production of Spanish heritage language (SHL) learners. 163 university students were divided into two types of basic—and intermediate—level SHL courses: one in which they received explicit instruction regarding accent marks (EI), and another in which they did not (non-EI). Findings suggest that the effectiveness of explicit instruction depends upon level of study and task type. In EI, the accuracy of first semester SHL students improved significantly, while their counterparts in non-EI course showed no appreciable gains. The case of intermediate-level learners, who had some prior formal study, was more variable. Although high-, mid-, and low-performing learners in second semester EI courses showed significant improvement in a dictation task (while their non-EI counterparts did not), performance on open-ended exam responses did not reflect significant improvement. In the analysis we consider task effects and within-group differences.

----

El presente estudio evaluó el impacto de la instrucción explícita en el uso del acento ortográfico en la escritura de estudiantes de herencia hispanohablante. Un total de 163 estudiantes universitarios se dividieron en dos tipos de cursos de nivel básico e intermedio de español como lengua de herencia: uno en el que recibieron instrucción explícita (IE) sobre el uso del acento escrito y otro en el que no la recibieron (sin IE). Los resultados sugieren que el efecto de la instrucción explícita depende del nivel de estudio y del tipo de tarea. En las secciones con IE, la precisión de los estudiantes de español como lengua de herencia de primer semestre mejoró de modo significativo a lo largo del semestre, a diferencia de sus pares en las secciones sin IE quienes parecieron no mejorar. El caso de los estudiantes de nivel intermedio, quienes habían estudiado la lengua previamente, fue más variable. Aunque los estudiantes de alto, medio y bajo rendimiento en los cursos con IE de segundo semestre mostraron una mejora significativa en una tarea de dictado (a diferencia de sus pares en el curso sin IE), parecieron no mejorar en respuestas a preguntas abiertas. El análisis tiene en cuenta los efectos del tipo de tarea así como las diferencias dentro de los grupos.

This study examines the effects of word frequency and typographical enhancement (i.e., words in bold) on the learning of single words in Spanish L2 reading activities in a classroom setting. Learners read a text in which target words appearing in bold occurred one, five or 10 times. A parallel text was also provided in which those same words appeared un-bolded. Students were tested three times on both spelling and meaning of the target words: before, immediately after and two weeks after the reading activity. Results show that: (1) after five or 10 encounters, both spelling and meaning recognition skills increase; (2) words appearing in bold only lead to learning gains at a form recognition level immediately after the test is taken and do not contribute to the acquisition of meaning in the post-test; and (3) there is no interaction between typographical enhancement and frequency.

-----------------

Este trabajo pretende examinar los efectos de la frecuencia y el realce tipográfico (negrita) en el aprendizaje de palabras individuales a través de la lectura en el aula de español como L2. Los aprendientes leyeron un texto con palabras meta que aparecían 1, 5 o 10 veces. Este tenía a su vez una versión paralela en la cual dichas palabras aparecían en negrita. Los estudiantes fueron examinados en el reconocimiento de la forma y del significado de las palabras meta en tres ocasiones: antes, inmediatamente después y dos semanas después de la actividad de lectura. Los resultados demostraron que (1) el reconocimiento de la forma y del significado se incrementa cuando la palabra aparece repetida 5 o 10 veces; (2) el realce únicamente favorece el reconocimiento ortográfico inmediatamente después de la realización de la prueba y no contribuye al aprendizaje del significado; y (3) no existe interacción entre frecuencia y realce.

The imagination effect occurs when students learn better from imagining concepts and procedures rather than from studying them. Cognitive load theory explains the effect by better use of available working memory resources and increased productive, intrinsic cognitive load. The effect has been found in numerous empirical studies. However, in the majority of studies demonstrating the effect, visual/spatial information has been used: the type of information that is believed to be easy for imagining. The reported two experiments investigated if an imagination effect could be obtained using verbal information in the area of learning a second language. The results indicated that while textual material was expectedly more difficult for learning than diagrammatic material, asking learners to imagine textual material did improve performance. Cognitive load theory has been used as a theoretical framework to interpret the results.

Texto completo pdf icon

According to Hopp’s Lexical Bottleneck Hypothesis, difficulties in second language (L2) lexical processing may lead to non-target syntactic computations. In line with this hypothesis, cognates – which are processed faster than non-cognates, as defined by the cognate facilitation effect – can ease L2 syntactic processing. In order to investigate whether cognates additionally facilitate L2 syntax learning, we had two groups of Spanish natives learn an artificial language drawing on Basque. Each group explicitly learnt a set of either Spanish–Basque cognates or non-cognates and a word order grammatical rule. Then, two sentence–picture matching tasks tested their ability to apply the rule (1) with cognates vs. non-cognates and (2) with novel cognate words. Results showed that, in both cases, cognate learners were better at applying the rule than non-cognate learners. This finding suggests that the cognate facilitation effect and its role in the Lexical Bottleneck Hypothesis can be extended from L2 processing to L2 learning. We end by mentioning possible implications of our results for second language teaching in adulthood.

This study explores the impact of direct and indirect feedback on the improvement of Spanish past aspect (imperfect/preterit) in writing activities in a third-semester Spanish course. Ninety learners were divided into three groups: direct feedback, indirect feedback, and control (no feedback). Learners completed a pretest, a posttest, and a delayed posttest. Although it remains unclear which type of feedback is more effective, results indicate that both types improved preterit–imperfect performance. As the delayed posttest showed, however, learners exhibited more lasting improvement (as indicated by the delayed posttest) when working on the production Activity than on the terminology and selection activities.

--------------------

Este estudio explora el impacto de la retroalimentación directa e indirecta en la mejora del aspecto de pasado (pretérito/imperfecto) con actividades de escritura en un curso de tercer semestre de español. Se dividió noventa estudiantes en tres grupos: retroalimentación directa, retroalimentación indirecta, y un grupo de control (sin retroalimentación). Completaron un pretest, postest, y postest tardío. Aunque no hubo diferencias significativas entre los grupos experimentales, los resultados indican que los participantes mejoraron en el uso de aspecto verbal. No obstante, fue una mejora más perdurable (postest tardío) en la actividad de producción que en las actividades de terminología y selección.

Research on pre-task planning to date has mainly focused on task performance. However, the effects of planning are contingent on what learners actually do during planning time. One important factor that may determine the quality and usefulness of planning is whether it is done in the first language (L1) or the second language (L2). This research addresses this issue by investigating the relative benefits of collaborative planning in the L1 and L2 in terms of ideas generated and transferred to an oral problem-solving task. Seventy-two Japanese university EFL learners were randomly assigned to one of two planning conditions: L1P (L1 planning, Japanese) and L2P (L2 planning, English). Dyads in each group were given 10 minutes to plan the content of a problem-solving task in the respective languages before individually performing the timed 2.5-minute oral task. Data took the form of transcribed planning discussions and transcribed task performances. All data were coded for idea units and sorted into categories of problem–solution discourse structure (situation, problem, response, evaluation). A qualitative comparison of L1 and L2 planners’ generation of idea units during planning, transfer and performance was conducted to supplement the quantitative analysis. Findings indicate the L1P condition has significant advantages over the L2P condition in terms of idea conceptualization, but this advantage had a limited impact on subsequent L2 task performance. Pedagogical implications are discussed in terms of possibilities for productively incorporating L1 planning during task implementation in foreign language contexts where learners share a common first language.

Recent research indicated the effectiveness of immediate and delayed corrective feedback (CF) on second language (L2) learning. What is little known is the moderating effects of learner individual factors such as foreign language anxiety (FLA) on the efficacy of immediate and delayed CF. The primary aim of the present study is thus to investigate if learners’ FLA can moderate the effectiveness of immediate and delayed feedback. To this end, 82 learners of English as a foreign language (EFL) were randomly assigned to two treatment conditions as well as a control group. During three treatment sessions, the participants performed storytelling tasks in groups of four and received either immediate or delayed metalinguistic feedback for the errors they made. Regarding the immediate feedback condition, each participant received metalinguistic feedback immediately following an error while doing the storytelling task. For the delayed feedback condition, metalinguistic feedback was offered for each error at the end of each storytelling task. Two testing instruments, a writing task and an error correction test were used as pretest, posttest and delayed posttest to measure learners’ development as a result of the treatment sessions. Without considering learners’ FLA, the findings revealed that immediate and delayed CF are equally effective to promote L2 development. However, when taking into consideration learners’ FLA through regression analysis, the results revealed that immediate CF was more beneficial for learners with low FLA compared to high anxiety learners, but no significant associations were found between FLA and delayed CF efficacy.

Texto completo PDF icon

In this article we explore the role of linguistic landscapes, which refers to language on display in public spaces, in the teaching of languages and enhancing language awareness. Signage can be useful for language learners as a pedagogical tool for language acquisition and to explore issues of multilingualism.

We focus in particular on the multilingual education context in the Basque Country in Spain, where the three languages of instruction are Basque, Spanish and English. Our analysis is based on data collected in public spaces, from students in primary schools and masters-level students at university. Our data include signage in a local covered market, and on the walls of schools as well as that collected among students who carried out learning tasks investigating the signage that surrounds them. We conclude that the languages on display in public spaces are an important resource for language learning and teaching, and they can also be used for raising language awareness.

-----

En este artículo exploramos el papel de los paisajes lingüísticos, que se refiere al lenguaje que se exhibe en los espacios públicos, en la enseñanza de lenguas y en la mejora de la conciencia lingüística. La señalización puede ser útil para los estudiantes de lenguas como herramienta pedagógica para la adquisición de lenguas y para explorar cuestiones relacionadas con el multilingüismo. Nos centramos en particular en el contexto de la educación multilingüe en el País Vasco en España, donde los tres idiomas de instrucción son el euskera, el español y el inglés. La base de nuestro análisis son los datos que hemos recopilado en ubicaciones en espacios públicos, dentro de las escuelas y con alumnos de primaria y estudiantes de máster en la universidad. Nuestros datos incluyen la señalización de un edificio que contiene un mercado local, paredes de escuelas, así como los datos recogidos entre los alumnos que realizaron tareas de aprendizaje investigando la señalización que los rodea. Concluimos que las lenguas que se exhiben en los espacios públicos son un recurso importante para el aprendizaje y la enseñanza de idiomas y también pueden usarse para desarrollar la conciencia lingüística.

This classroom-based action research (CBAR) corroborated our belief in the valuable role rubrics play in a tertiary L2 writing context where English is the medium of instruction. The three-stage CBAR involved ongoing discussions between us, two writing teacher-researchers, as we adapted our teaching and assessment strategies to explore the potential of rubrics as formative tools. This study confirmed the proactive role rubrics could play in teaching writing and promoting successful partnerships between teachers and students during the assessment process. The multifaceted function of rubrics as driver of change in practitioners’ approaches to teaching and assessing writing as well as a tool that enables students to take ownership of the different stages of their writing was a major finding of our study.

Texto completo pdf icon

Glossing is a widely used and examined vocabulary learning tool, and one of the major branches of glossing research has compared the relative effects of first language (L1) and second language (L2) glosses on reading comprehension and vocabulary learning. However, the findings in this literature have not been consistent, calling for a comprehensive and systematic review. To this end, we conducted a meta-analysis to investigate the relative effects of L1 and L2 glossing on L2 reading comprehension and L2 vocabulary learning. Based on 78 effect sizes gathered from 26 studies representing 30 independent samples (N = 2,189), we found that L1 glossing was more effective than L2 glossing in general (Hedge’s g = .33, SE = .09, p < .001), but the effect size may vary depending on the target outcome measure. The relative effectiveness of L1 glossing was particularly supported by the results of immediate posttests of vocabulary, rather than delayed posttests of vocabulary and reading comprehension tests. Further, among a few selected moderator variables, the results of meta-regression revealed that learners’ L2 proficiency level significantly influenced the average effectiveness, such that L1 glossing is particularly effective for beginner learners compared to those with intermediate or higher L2 proficiency levels.

Texto completo
DOI: https://doi.org/10.5209/clac.76713

El presente artículo explora la aplicación de la metonimia conceptual (Lakoff, 1987; Ruiz de Mendoza, 2000; Langacker, 2009; Panther, Thornburg y Barcelona, 2009; Barcelona 2013) en el desarrollo de materiales pedagógicos para la enseñanza del sistema verbal a aprendientes de español como segunda lengua/lengua extranjera. Para ello, se describen las ventajas de introducir el razonamiento inferencial mediante la metonimia en los materiales didácticos —descripciones y actividades—, a partir de algunos de los principios del modelo de la Gramática Cognitiva (Langacker, 1987, 1991, 2000, 2001, 2008, 2009). El artículo se centra en los usos de tiempos verbales que implican procesos metonímicos, en concreto, las extensiones de significado presentes en dos tiempos pretéritos en español: (1) el significado accional de verbos estativos cuando se transmiten con pretérito indefinido, como en Pudimos comprar la casa, que se ve ampliado metonímicamente a Compramos la casa; y (2) el uso distanciador (citativo) del pretérito imperfecto referido a eventos o situaciones presentes, como en ¿Cómo te llamabas?, ampliado metonímicamente a Se me ha dicho/No recuerdo/No he oído cómo te llamas. Por último, con el fin de analizar en qué medida este enfoque puede contribuir a la enseñanza del sistema verbal español, se explora el potencial pedagógico de algunas técnicas y recursos, en concreto, (a) descripciones gramaticales pedagógicas centradas en el razonamiento metafórico y metonímico, (b) ejercicios de paráfrasis centrados en la indeterminación del significado para promover la concienciación de los aprendientes, y (c) la creación de redes y el uso de imágenes (dinámicas) para ejemplificar la variabilidad de las conceptualizaciones del significado gramatical

---------------------------------

This paper explores the application of conceptual metonymy (Lakoff, 1987; Ruiz de Mendoza, 2000; Langacker, 2009; Panther, Thornburg and Barcelona, 2009; Barcelona, 2013) in the development of pedagogical resources for the teaching of the Spanish verbal system to L2/FL learners. To this end, a description is given of the advantages of introducing inferential reasoning using metonymy based on certain principles taken from the Cognitive Grammar model (Langacker, 1987, 1991, 2000, 2001, 2008, 2009) in grammar teaching materials — both descriptions and activities. We focus on tense uses that involve metonymic processes, particularly, the meaning extensions in two Spanish past tenses: (1) the actional meaning of stative verbs when conveyed in pretérito indefinido (preterit), as in Pudimos comprar la casa 'We could buy the house', metonymically extended to Compramos la casa 'We bought the house'; and (2) the distancing use (uso citativo) of pretérito imperfecto (imperfect) when referring to current facts, as in ¿Cómo te llamabas? (How PRO.REFL.2SG call.IPFV.PST.2SG?) 'What was your name again?', which is metonymically extended to (Se me ha dicho/No recuerdo/No he oído) cómo te llamas [('I have been told/I can't recall/I couldn't hear') how PRO.REFL.2SG call.PRES.2SG]. In order to discuss the kind of contribution that this conceptual standpoint can make to the teaching of the Spanish verbal system, the pedagogical potential of some techniques and resources is explored in terms of (a) metonymic and metaphorical reasoning in pedagogical grammar descriptions, (b) consciousness-raising paraphrase exercises focused on meaning indeterminacy, and (c) network building and the use of (dynamic) images to show variable construals in grammatical meaning.

Translation for language learning has reemerged during recent years as an additional asset for foreign language instruction. In this time, the professional practice and training of translation has moved to a technological paradigm in which virtually all translation activities are computer mediated and this complex activity is conceptualized as human–computer interaction. Translation is therefore now inseparable from the physical computer, translation technologies, tools and resources found online and the interconnectedness provided by the Internet. This article reviews these technologies in the context of Spanish language learning. For these purposes, three main areas of interest are discussed and placed in the context of language learning. First of all, the role of machine translation (MT) both as an assistance in reading and writing tasks and from the perspective of post-editing for language learning is examined. The second area of interest is the use of translational corpora in the foreign language classroom. The article will finish with a review of the increasingly popular language-related crowdsourcing technologies that are expanding translation tasks to language learners around the world using specialized micro-task workflows.

---------------------

La traducción para el aprendizaje de idiomas ha resurgido en los últimos años como un activo adicional para el aprendizaje de idiomas. Durante estos años, la práctica profesional y la formación de la traducción han evolucionado hacia un paradigma tecnológico en el que prácticamente todas las actividades de traducción son mediadas por ordenador y esta compleja actividad es conceptualizada como interacción hombre-computadora. La traducción es por lo tanto inseparable del ordenador, las tecnologías de traducción, las herramientas y los recursos encontrados en línea y la interconexión que proporciona la WWW. Este artículo examina estas tecnologías en el contexto del aprendizaje del español. Con este objetivo se repasan tres áreas principales de interés en el contexto del aprendizaje de idiomas. En primer lugar, se examina el papel de la traducción automática como ayuda en las tareas de lectura y escritura y desde la perspectiva de su posedición para el aprendizaje del idioma. La segunda área de interés es el uso de corpus de traducciones en las clases de idiomas. El artículo finalizará con una revisión de las tecnologías de crowdsourcing lingüístico que están ganando popularidad y que extienden las tareas de traducción a estudiantes de idiomas de todo el mundo utilizando flujos de trabajo especializados basados en microtareas.

Texto completo pdf icon

This article presents the Spanish version of the Computer-Assisted Listening and Speaking Tutor (CALST), an online platform that can be used to complement pronunciation training in the classroom. The Spanish CALST offers listening, speaking, and spelling exercises for Northern-Central Peninsular Spanish as an L2. Exercises are tailored by an automatic comparison with the learner’s native language based on a database of phonetically specified phoneme inventories for over 500 languages, with the result that learners with different L1s are exposed to different exercises adapted to their specific needs. In this article, we present a description of the exercises as well as the criteria used to develop Spanish content for CALST. We discuss the limitations of the platform, the logging of user results as a partial solution to these limitations, and the possible future use of the logged data to increase our understanding of L2 acquisition.

Pages

Revistas